Understanding neurocognitive recovery in older adults after total hip arthroplasty—neurocognitive assessment, blood biomarkers and patient experiences: a mixed-methods study =============================================================================================================================================================================== * Anahita Amirpour * Lina Bergman * Gabriela Markovic * Karin Liander * Ulrica Nilsson * Jeanette Eckerblad ## Abstract **Objective** Delayed neurocognitive recovery, previously known as postoperative cognitive dysfunction, is a common complication affecting older adults after surgery. This study aims to address the knowledge gap in postoperative neurocognitive recovery by exploring the relationship between subjective experiences, performance-based measurements, and blood biomarkers. **Design** Mixed-methods study with a convergent parallel (QUAL+quan) design. **Setting and participants** The study reports results from 40 older adult patients (52.5% women; mean age 73, SD 6.7) scheduled for total hip arthroplasty at a hospital in Sweden. **Outcome measures** Neurocognitive performance was assessed using a standardised test battery, neuroinflammation through blood biomarker analysis and postoperative neurocognitive recovery via semistructured interviews and the Swedish Quality of Recovery questionnaire. **Results** Five patients were classified as having delayed neurocognitive recovery based on performance tests. Qualitative data revealed that most patients reported cognitive symptoms, particularly related to executive functions and fatigue. Psychological factors, including a sense of agency and low mood, significantly influenced cognitive recovery and daily functioning. Elevated inflammatory blood biomarkers were not detected pre- or postoperatively in patients with delayed neurocognitive recovery. The global postoperative recovery score was 40.9, indicating a low quality of recovery. **Conclusion** Many patients reported subjective cognitive decline that was not corroborated by delayed neurocognitive recovery in the performance-based tests. Psychological factors were influential for neurocognitive recovery and should be routinely assessed. Future research should incorporate longitudinal follow-ups with performance-based measurements, fatigue assessment, evaluations of instrumental activities of daily living and subjective reporting, supported by a multidisciplinary team approach. **Trial registration number** [NCT05361460](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?link_type=CLINTRIALGOV&access_num=NCT05361460&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom). * NEUROLOGY * ORTHOPAEDIC & TRAUMA SURGERY * Old age psychiatry ### STRENGTHS AND LIMITATIONS OF THIS STUDY * To our knowledge, this is the first mixed-methods study exploring performance-based measurements and subjective reports of postoperative neurocognitive recovery after orthopaedic surgery. * We assessed neurocognitive performance with a test battery, explored postoperative neurocognitive recovery through semistructured interviews and measured the potential neuroinflammatory response with blood biomarkers. * Results from 40 patients at a university hospital in Sweden are presented, a sample that may not be generalisable to other contexts. ## Introduction Delayed neurocognitive recovery (dNCR), formerly known as postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD),1 commonly affects older adults within a month post surgery2 3 Neuroinflammation and oxidative stress have been demonstrated to be a part of the mechanism of dNCR,4 5 and proinflammatory cytokines such as interleukin (IL)-6 and tumour necrosis factor α (TNF-α) enter the brain via normal or disrupted blood-brain barrier.6 Yet, at present there are no specific inflammatory biomarkers clinically validated for predicting or diagnosing dNCR.5 Moreover, fluctuations in tryptophan plasma levels have been suggested as a potential cause of postoperative fatigue, affecting serotonin 5-HT production and contributing to postoperative fatigue through 5-HT-synthesis resulting from changes in plasma amino acid levels.7 The recovery process from surgery is a multifaceted construct influenced by physical, psychological and social factors.8 Patients may regain their preoperative state or surpass it, reaching a high level of well-being and recovering lost functions.8 9 While perioperative research has emphasised overall recovery, the understanding of neurocognitive recovery in particular—what it entails, how it is experienced and its implications—remains ambiguous. dNCR manifests with cognitive decline in memory, attention, processing speed and executive functions,1 and is linked to heightened disability risk.10 Traditionally, dNCR was assessed only through neurocognitive tests,11 but the updated nomenclature includes subjective cognitive decline (SCD) and daily function changes in the diagnosis.1 SCD, reported even without cognitive impairment, indicates elevated future cognitive impairment and dementia risk.12 However, perioperative research has primarily focused on quantitative measures of dNCR in the past decades, resulting in subjective reports being overlooked. Therefore, this mixed-methods study aims to fill the current knowledge gap in postoperative neurocognitive recovery by integrating quantitative and qualitative data. By exploring performance-based measurements (neurocognitive test battery), blood samples (biomarkers) and how subjective reports on neurocognitive recovery (semistructured interviews and a patient-reported outcome) are experienced. We hypothesised that patients showing a decline in performance-based tests would have differing experiences in the interviews, and vice versa. ## Methods ### Study design and setting This mixed-methods study had a convergent parallel (QUAL+quan) design13 14 and was conducted at a university hospital in Stockholm, Sweden. The mixed-methods design was qualitatively dominant, and we integrated both qualitative and quantitative approaches15 with the intention to provide an in-depth understanding of early postoperative neurocognitive recovery, following the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental disorders fifth edition criteria for mild/major neurocognitive disorders.16 We collected and analysed the quantitative and qualitative data separately, subsequently merged to identify any convergences, divergences or relationships between the two. We obtained ethical permit (2019-02968) from the Swedish Ethical Review Authority on 19 June 2019, registered the study at ClinicalTrials.gov ([NCT05361460](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?link_type=CLINTRIALGOV&access_num=NCT05361460&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom)), and published the study protocol.17 We recruited patients at the scheduled clinical preoperative visit, provided study information and obtained written informed consent from all patients, following the Declaration of Helsinki.18 ### Study population Between October 2019 and November 2021, we included 46 patients aged ≥60 years through convenience sampling. There were six dropouts (figure 1). Recruitment was extended by 18 months due to the COVID-19 outbreak. All potential eligible study participants were preliminary screened and approached by the fourth author. The patients were scheduled for total hip arthroplasty, and all patients underwent both the quantitative and qualitative data collection. Exclusion criteria were Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE)19 ≤ 22, nervous system disease, dependence on antidepressant or tranquilliser, alcohol or drug misuse, hearing or visual impairment, surgery in the previous 6 months and lack of fluency in Swedish. ![Figure 1](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/https://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/bmjopen/15/1/e093872/F1.medium.gif) [Figure 1](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/15/1/e093872/F1) Figure 1 Flowchart of participants. ### Outcome measures We obtained demographic and perioperative data from patient records, including comorbidities, age, sex, MMSE score, pain intensity with numeric rating scale, education level, cohabitant status, American Society of Anesthesiologists classification, anaesthetic technique and duration, and duration of the surgery. ### Neurocognitive assessment We measured neurocognitive performance with the International Study of Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction (ISPOCD) test battery,20 administered by the fourth author, who was trained in neurocognitive testing. The battery includes four neurocognitive tests20: 1. **Visual Verbal Learning Test (VVLT**) measuring verbal episodic memory, based on Rey’s auditive recall of words, includes 15 words in three trials. A higher total word count indicates better scores. 2. **Concept Shifting Task (CST)**, measuring visual mental flexibility, based on the Trail Making Test includes 16 circles in three trials. Less time and fewer errors indicate better scores. 3. **Letter-Digit Coding Test (LDC**), measuring executive attention, working memory and speed, based on the Symbol Digits Substitution Test during 60 s. High scores indicate better performance. 4. **Stroop Colour-Word Test (SCW**), measuring executive selective attention, includes 40 words in three trials. Less time and fewer errors indicate better scoring. ### Patient-reported outcome measurement We assessed postoperative quality of recovery with the 24-item Swedish Quality of Recovery questionnaire (SwQoR-24). Each item measures various symptoms or discomfort related to surgery and anaesthesia such as pain, nausea, anxiety, sleep difficulties and fatigue. The patient rates these items on an 11-item scale, ranging from 0 (indicating none of the time) to 10 (indicating all the time). The range is from 0, indicating excellent quality of postoperative recovery, to 240, indicating poor quality of postoperative recovery. The patient is considered to have a good postoperative recovery if they have a global score less than 21 on postoperative day 14. The SwQoR-24 has been validated in a Swedish setting with postoperative patients.21 22 ### Bloodborne biomarkers We measured inflammatory biomarkers granulocyte-macrophage colony-stimulating factor (GM-CSF), interferon-ɣ (IFN-ɣ), IL-2, IL-4, IL-6, IL-8, IL-10 and TNF-α, and the non-inflammatory biomarker tryptophan at the preoperative visit, postoperative day 1 and on days 13–16 to assess its association with neurocognitive recovery. We took peripheral blood (11 mL whole blood) from the patient, centrifuged it and plasma was stored at −80°C until analysis. Tryptophan was measured using the standardised technique high-performance liquid chromatography. We analysed all blood samples in August 2023, with the Bio-Rad Bio-Plex Pro Human Cytokine 8-plex Assay #M50000007A. ### Procedure The preoperative assessment at the orthopaedic clinic included a performance-based measurement using a standardised test battery (ISPOCD), blood sampling and SwQoR-24.The postoperative assessment on days 1–3 included blood sampling and SwQoR-24. On days 13–16, the postoperative assessment at the orthopaedic clinic included the test battery, blood sampling, SwQoR-24 and semistructured qualitative interviews. This timeline was selected to capture dNCR, which is manifested within 30 days after surgery.23 ### Surgery and anaesthesia The total hip arthroplasty surgery was carried out in accordance with normal clinical practice. Patients received spinal anaesthesia, either with 0.25 mL morphine (0.4 mg mL) and 2.8 mL bupivacaine (5 mg mL) at level L3–L4 or L2-L3, or with 3.5 mL bupivacaine (5 mg mL) only. Four patients underwent general anaesthesia with tracheal intubation using a combination of induction drugs such as alfentanil, propofol, fentanyl and a variation of neuromuscular-blocking drugs, and maintenance anaesthesia with sevoflurane. ### Qualitative data Semistructured, face-to-face interviews were conducted 2 weeks after surgery. The interview questions covered cognitive functions, daily activities and overall mood, following an interview guide (online supplemental material 2). Each interview was audio-recorded and transcribed verbatim. ### Supplementary data [[bmjopen-2024-093872supp002.pdf]](pending:yes) ### Data analysis #### Statistical analysis Descriptive statistics are presented as means, SD, median score and completion times for the neurocognitive test battery. Wilcoxon signed-rank test was applied to assess changes in raw scores and completion times for the neurocognitive test battery. Normality of the data was assessed with Q-Q plots, histograms and Shapiro-Wilk test. A two-sided p-value of <0.05 was considered statistically significant. Cognitive performance changes were adjusted for practice effects and variability using age-matched nonsurgical controls; the z-scores were calculated to assess changes from preoperative to postoperative tests, with dNCR defined as a z-score of ≥1.0 on days 13–16 after surgery and z-score of <1.0 on days 13–16 indicated no decline according to the ISPOCD method.20 We followed the diagnostic rule for dNCR, meaning a decline in at least two subtests.11 We used IBM SPSS V.28 (IBM Corp, Armonk, New York, USA) for statistical analysis. #### Qualitative analysis Four authors (AA, GM, JE, LB) analysed the qualitative data. We applied Elo and Kyngäs’24 description of content analysis to the data, with a deductive and inductive approach. We initially chose a set of categories, that is, cognitive domains; attention and memory and executive functions based on theoretical framework,25 and our research objectives. These categories served as a structured matrix to code the data. As our analysis advanced, we recognised a recurring affective theme in the interviews. We openly coded these meaning units and categorised them as psychological factors, aligning with our research questions and acknowledging their influence on neurocognitive recovery. The analysis process involved several iterative steps. First, we read the verbatim transcribed interviews thoroughly. Then, we developed a structured categorisation matrix (online supplemental material 1) and reviewed and coded the data according to the categories and subcategories, and only extracted data that fit the final matrix.24 Finally, we held meetings regularly within our research group to achieve an agreement on data analysis. ### Supplementary data [[bmjopen-2024-093872supp001.pdf]](pending:yes) #### Mixed-methods analysis First, we analysed the qualitative and quantitative data sets separately. Then, we merged the results from the data sets by conducting a thorough side-by-side comparison, which is visualised in the joint display (table 1).26 The joint display comparison enabled us to assess for confirmation, discordance and expansion of the data sets, and draw meta-inferences.26 All findings were discussed within the research group. The initial proposed display was created by AA through an iterative process, with patterns, revisions and reviews conducted by LB and GM. View this table: [Table 1](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/15/1/e093872/T1) Table 1 Joint display presenting quantitative, qualitative and mixed-methods meta-inferences of domains #### Patient and public involvement Patients and the public were not involved in the design, conduct, reporting or dissemination of this research. ## Results This section starts with patient characteristics based on assessment data including biomarkers, followed by domain-level findings on executive functions, attention and memory, and psychological factors. In the ‘Discussion’ section, the integrated results are further expanded through a joint display (table 1). ### Patient characteristics and perioperative data Six patients were excluded from the analyses because of withdrawn participation, postoperative complications, surgery elsewhere and the research team not being available, thus leaving 40 patients (figure 1). Patient characteristics and test results on a group level are presented in tables 2 and 3. View this table: [Table 2](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/15/1/e093872/T2) Table 2 Patient characteristics View this table: [Table 3](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/15/1/e093872/T3) Table 3 Summary of the patients’ raw scores and completion times on the neurocognitive tests ### Neurocognitive assessment Among the 40 patients, 5 were classified as dNCR (z-score >1.0 in at least two subtests), with no statistical differences in anaesthetic factors or characteristics between those with/without dNCR. The mean scores and relevant completion times for each sub-test are presented in table 3. ### Patient-reported quality of recovery On postoperative day 14, the patients’ postoperative recovery global score was mean 40.9 (table 2), indicating low quality of recovery. There were no differences in SwQoR-24 scores between those with/without dNCR. ### Bloodborne biomarkers One patient did not have a preoperative inflammatory biomarker result, and three patients had missing results on the first postoperative day. The cytokines GM-CSF, IFN-γ, IL-10, IL-2 and IL-4 were undetectable in all patients, while IL-6, IL-8 and TNF-α were detectable but below 0 pg/mL. Tryptophan levels (table 2) were low both preoperatively and postoperatively in the total sample. ### Executive functions Among the participants, n=12/40 declined on the SCW test, n=10/40 on the CST test and n=8/40 on the LDC test (table 1). Moreover, in the interviews (online supplemental material 1), the most significant and frequent problems the patients described were related to their executive functions. The main qualitative findings were associated to *problem-solving, emotional regulation, energisation and fatigue*. These challenges manifested considerably when patients tried to resume their everyday activities at home or work, including meal preparation and initiating social contacts. While some patients dedicated effort to their physical rehabilitation, others refrained due to energy constraints, recounting days spent entirely in bed. The patients described developing new strategies and skills to deal with the current changed form, where some patients learnt to carry the mug in another way, using a basket to carry the plate with food to the bed to eat, using a ladder with help from spouse, or if the patient was living alone, this also affected their strategy, doing the task independently: > And I have learned to walk, so that it works. But it was an effort I didn't think I would have to make. But it was the first time in these 50 years that I feel strained. (P01). The effort to sustain energy to certain activities became particularly apparent in patients living with spouses or children as these family members assumed every task, from dressing to household chores. Patients struggling with these limitations often experienced emotional turmoil, expressing anger, impatience and frustration on realising their changed capacity for simple everyday tasks: > I've been a bit grumpy, I guess. I don't need to hide that. But no one has taken offense. > > I've tried to be kind and nice, but sometimes you just snap a bit. (P10). These issues with regulating emotions were previously unfamiliar to the patients and sometimes led to strained relationships, as some patients vented their emotions on their spouses. The patients’ coping mechanisms varied, with some patients testing how far they could go in attempting presurgery activities such as leaving the house and go on a walk. Conversely, others embraced their current limitations, recognising the futility of certain tasks during this phase of recovery. The patients conveyed a profound sense of fatigue or lethargy, irrespective of what they did or following specific activities. This fatigue was articulated on either cognitive and motoric domain, or both: > The only thing I've managed is to go to the bathroom and take care of my needs and … yes, brush my teeth and things like that. […] I can handle such tasks, but nothing else. I don’t have the energy for it, I’m too tired. […] I couldn't even dress myself at first. My husband had to help me get dressed, you know. (P06). In response to this fatigue, patients adopted alternative coping mechanisms. Some patients resorted to daytime sleeping while others avoided activities. This avoidance, distinct from their presurgical behaviour, was characterised by patients refraining from planned activities. For instance, they reported a shift from an intention to tidy up the house to do nothing at all. Similarly, they described avoiding interactions with friends or family members due to a lack of energy to engage in conversations. ### Attention and memory In the VVLT, n=4/40 patients declined. The main qualitative findings were related to *subjective or family concerns of memory decline, sustained attention and mind wandering*. Patients frequently described instances of forgetfulness, such as entering the kitchen or bathroom and subsequently forgetting their intended tasks. Some explicitly acknowledged memory decline, recognising pre-existing issues even before surgery. Patients who experienced forgetfulness occasionally questioned themselves, speculating whether such lapses existed before surgery. > Yes, I feel like I've had a really poor memory for a long time now. Because I've been anxious about the surgery, and that affects concentration a bit. And I haven't been feeling very well before either. (P07). Others recognised their memory decline to ageing. For example, one patient expressed family concern, revealing that a family member had commented on his memory loss recently. As a result, the family member had taken over tasks the patient once handled independently. Consequently, the patient articulated he perceived a memory loss. Some patients described how their minds wandered, especially during activities like reading or showering, leading to difficulties in sustaining their attention. As a result, they often abandoned the task. In contrast, others created adaptive strategies to manage their focus and memory, such as preplanning their medication routine and organising pills in specific containers. ### Psychological factors The main qualitative findings were related to *sense of agency, powerlessness, physical limitations and future perspectives*. In the interviews, some patients expressed a sense of relief and improved well-being post surgery, assigning it to the resolution of long-term pain that had accompanied every movement before surgery. This positive change had a notable impact on their mood as they reflected on their presurgery state characterised by persistent pain. > I feel much more positive now than right after the surgery, as I sense that the pain is heading in the right direction, and the mobility in the operated leg also feels much better, in that way. So, I feel that I am regaining a bit more zest for life compared to before the surgery. (P03). On the contrary, other patients conveyed feelings of powerlessness and dependence on family members post surgery, particularly in managing daily activities. Despite their family members' well-intentioned efforts to protect them, this gave rise to annoyance. The patients had a desire to maintain a sense of agency even though their abilities had changed post surgery. This transition from independent functioning to reliance on others resulted in feelings of despair or a bad mood: > … To 110%. I don't want to be dependent … Yes, I become disheartened and a little angry, and … What should I say? … Just this being dependent, it’s … Yes, I want to do everything myself if I may say so. Control my day, or control and manage and so on. (P08) Expectations for the future and the ability to function independently raised concerns, especially regarding the possibility of driving a car again. These worries about the future, coupled with doubts about improvement, led to mood disturbances such as irritability and feeling low. > I have a different way, a different temperament. I don't recognize myself. I am sometimes sad, and that’s not something I used to be. (P46) Several patients spoke about the significant shift from being in control presurgery to a postoperative state where they felt a loss of control over their bodies, their capabilities and a sense of being different. This perceived loss of control within the healthcare system left patients feeling exposed. ## Discussion We explored how 40 older adult patients experienced neurocognitive recovery after total hip arthroplasty and how this experience aligned or differed with neurocognitive assessment and biomarker results. Interestingly, no apparent differences were observed between those with detected dNCR and those without, whether in qualitative or quantitative data collection. Consequently, the data were presented at the group level. In the neurocognitive tests, only five patients were classified as dNCR. However, more patients showed impairments in individual cognitive domains. This indicates that, although they did not meet the criteria for dNCR, they still experienced some degree of cognitive impact. Among patients classified as having dNCR, no specific subjective complaints or expressions of worry were reported during the interviews. Moreover, SCD was widely expressed by many patients in the interviews. The incongruence between the performance-based measurements and SCD is anticipated27 as the controlled neurocognitive test environment lacks external distractions compared with home or the workplace. Patients may demonstrate normal cognitive performance briefly during the test, but their day-to-day functioning could be compromised, leading to SCD,28 which became noticeable in the qualitative data. Furthermore, our findings align with previous research that discovered no correlation between cognitive performance and self-reported cognitive complaints.2 28–30 Nonetheless, subjectively reported data in perioperative research are often obtained with a variability of methods, such as questionnaires,30 phone interviews post surgery29 31 or a single-item binary question.2 The variability in data gathering poses a substantial challenge in consolidating findings and identifying comprehensive patterns. The diverse definitions and measurement approaches for SCD further complicate this task. In contrast, ageing research on SCD has primarily focused on symptom type and intensity, with a higher symptom burden increasing the risk of clinical progression.32 We found no association between inflammatory biomarkers and dNCR, consistent with other studies.33–35 As the inflammatory biomarkers were either undetectable or below 0 pg/L, they were excluded from data integration. Previous studies vary in their results when using inflammatory biomarkers to detect dNCR; these variations may be due to different types of surgery and different methods of analysing inflammatory markers. For example, a meta-analysis33 revealed an association between elevated C reactive protein levels in both postoperative delirium and cognitive decline. However, insufficient evidence was available to draw conclusions regarding IL-6, while IL-8, IL-10 and TNF-α showed no significant association with cognitive decline. Similarly, a recent systematic review36 noted elevated IL-6 levels within <12 hours postoperatively in older adults but found no such association for TNF-α. Another study focusing on older adults after hip fracture surgery indicated that glucocorticoid administration reduced levels of IL-6 and TNF-α.37 Perioperative administration of glucocorticoids, commonly used in orthopaedic surgery, and non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, frequently prescribed for osteoarthritis,38 have been found to suppress cytokines, including IL-6 and TNF-α.39 Aligning with these findings, updated European guidelines on postoperative delirium advise against the use of biomarkers for the prediction or prevention of delirium in at-risk patients30. Nevertheless, it remains uncertain whether this recommendation extends to dNCR, posing implications of the design of future trials. Tryptophan levels were consistently low in our patients, similar to the findings in a study of patients with cancer-related fatigue.40 Interestingly, mean tryptophan levels in other studies were higher: 74.4 µM in patients with cancer-related fatigue41 and 65 µmol/L in bariatric surgery patients.42 Besides the serotonin pathway, tryptophan is catabolized in the kynurenine pathway and plays a role in energy homeostasis. Changes in this pathway can be associated with low-grade inflammation,41 which is relevant to our patient group with osteoarthritis, a chronic inflammatory condition. Postoperative fatigue, characterised by persistent weakness or tiredness, is frequently overlooked and significantly impacts cognitive, behavioural and physical functions, often delaying the resumptions of daily activities after surgery.43 We found that two of the most frequently described symptoms in the qualitative data were lack of energy and lethargy impacting the patient’s daily functioning after surgery, aligning with previous research.44 However, we did not ask how their energy levels were before the surgery. Lethargy and lack of energy may be interpreted as fatigue which is not traditionally a component in neurocognitive tests even though it impacts cognitive functioning. Assessment of postoperative fatigue can be a helpful element, in addition to neurocognitive assessments, to predict postoperative recovery. Furthermore, each meaning unit from the qualitative data may not exclusively correspond to a singular cognitive domain but can in fact match to more than one, such as subjective complaints about attention could match with memory. Previous literature indicates attention, working memory and executive control share substantial similarities in their functional and structural neural correlates.45 Postoperative pain was well-controlled, as evidenced by the low pain scores. While some patients described an improved sense of well-being after experiencing pain relief following surgery, others expressed concerns about the future and the ability to function independently. The different coping strategies which the patient employed to resume daily activities align with earlier studies.44 46 47 In our study, patients described low mood, dependency on others and perceived loss of agency which impacted their daily functioning due to the surgery. Earlier studies44 47–49 have found that psychological factors, specifically vulnerability factors like depressive and anxiety symptoms, affect postoperative recovery. These factors not only manifest behaviourally through avoidance behaviours but also have cognitive implications.50 Patients described how social support facilitated a smoother postoperative recovery process. Social support is related to improved global cognitive function, executive functions and memory.51 In our study, most patients resided with a spouse and received support in their daily activities, for example, help with dressing and cleaning. On the other hand, some patients expressed a sense of powerlessness due to their reliance on others. For them, depending on external support symbolised discomfort in seeking help. Therefore, clinical implications should include assessing surgical patients for emotional stress, such as depression and anxiety, as these factors are important predictors of postoperative recovery. Behavioural therapeutic interventions can be effective in addressing these concerns.52 The mean postoperative recovery score (SwQoR-24) was 40.9 on a group level, meaning they had a higher postoperative symptom burden and low quality of recovery, whereas a score <21 on postoperative 14, would indicate they had a good postoperative recovery.22 However, the quality of recovery score in the referenced study pertains to a day surgery unit with a mix of young and older patients, which may not be directly applicable to our group consisting of older adults with comorbidities. To our knowledge, this is the first mixed-methods study exploring dNCR together with psychological factors after total hip arthroplasty. All participants underwent qualitative interviews, blood tests and neurocognitive tests, and our results present detailed descriptions of postoperative neurocognitive and emotional recovery. We acknowledge the limitations of this study. These include strict eligibility criteria which led to the exclusion of many patients and may have excluded frailer individuals, for example, those with nervous system diseases. Generalisability of our results is limited due to a small number of participants, and the convenience sampling is also a limitation. Further, this study lacked a standardised delirium assessment while patients were at the hospital and a preoperative depression screening. However, the SwQoR-24 does include items assessing anxiety and depressive symptoms. While we acknowledge the potential for bias with the same person conducting both tests and interviews, efforts were made to minimise bias by standardising the test procedure and instructions provided to all participants. Future direction should involve multidisciplinary teams that bridge specialty, primary and social care services. Long-term follow-ups should include objective neurocognitive assessments, evaluations of fatigue and measurements of instrumental daily activities. Additionally, patients' subjective reports must be gathered in accordance with recommended terminology.1 ## Conclusion We found a disparity between subjective reports of neurocognitive recovery and performance-based measurements. Only five patients were classified as having dNCR; however, many patients described changes in their daily functioning due to cognitive and psychological symptoms. Our study highlights the complexity and breadth of postoperative neurocognitive recovery which extends beyond psychometric testing and blood samples. ## Data availability statement Data are available upon reasonable request. ## Ethics statements ### Patient consent for publication All participants received oral and written information about the study, and written consent was obtained from all participants. ### Ethics approval We obtained ethical permit (2019-02968) from the Swedish Ethical Review Authority on 19 June 2019. Participants gave informed consent to participate in the study before taking part. ## Acknowledgments We thank Jennifer Kanon, Professor Lars. I Eriksson, Professor Lars Rasmussen, Professor Olav Rooijackers, Dr Harald Brismar and all healthcare professionals at the hospital for their assistance. Extra thanks to the patients who participated in our study. ## Footnotes * X @LinaBergman * Contributors Guarantor and conceptualisation: UN. Methodology: AA, JE, LB, UN. Formal analysis; writing—review and editing: all authors. Investigation: KL. Data curation: AA, JE, LB, KL, UN. Writing—original draft: AA, JE, UN. Supervision: GM, LB, JE, UN. Project administration: AA, KL, UN. Funding acquisition: JE, LB, UN. * Funding This work was supported by The Research school in Health science, Karolinska Institutet grant number [2020-02641]; and Strategic Research Area Health Care Science, Karolinska Institutet grant number [2-1742/2021]. * Patient and public involvement Patients and/or the public were not involved in the design, or conduct, or reporting, or dissemination plans of this research. * Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed. * Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise. [https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/](https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Unported (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits others to copy, redistribute, remix, transform and build upon this work for any purpose, provided the original work is properly cited, a link to the licence is given, and indication of whether changes were made. See: [https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/](https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). ## References 1. Evered L , Silbert B , Knopman DS , et al . Recommendations for the Nomenclature of Cognitive Change Associated with Anaesthesia and Surgery-2018. Anesthesiology 2018;129:872–9. [doi:10.1097/ALN.0000000000002334](http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ALN.0000000000002334) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/ALN.0000000000002334&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=30325806&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 2. Deiner S , Liu X , Lin H-M , et al . Subjective cognitive complaints in patients undergoing major non-cardiac surgery: a prospective single centre cohort trial. Br J Anaesth 2019;122:742–50. [doi:10.1016/j.bja.2019.02.027](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bja.2019.02.027) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.bja.2019.02.027&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=31003631&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 3. Evered L , Atkins K , Silbert B , et al . Acute peri-operative neurocognitive disorders: a narrative review. Anaesthesia 2022;77:34–42. [doi:10.1111/anae.15613](http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/anae.15613) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=35001385&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 4. Safavynia SA , Goldstein PA . The Role of Neuroinflammation in Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction: Moving From Hypothesis to Treatment. Front Psychiatry 2018;9:752. [doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00752](http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00752) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=30705643&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 5. Evered LA , Silbert BS . Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction and Noncardiac Surgery. Anesthesia & Analgesia 2018;127:496–505. [doi:10.1213/ANE.0000000000003514](http://dx.doi.org/10.1213/ANE.0000000000003514) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=29889707&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 6. Li Z , Zhu Y , Kang Y , et al . Neuroinflammation as the Underlying Mechanism of Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction and Therapeutic Strategies. Front Cell Neurosci 2022;16:843069. [doi:10.3389/fncel.2022.843069](http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2022.843069) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=35418837&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 7. McGuire J , Ross GL , Price H , et al . Biochemical markers for post-operative fatigue after major surgery. Brain Res Bull 2003;60:125–30. [doi:10.1016/s0361-9230(03)00021-2](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0361-9230(03)00021-2) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0361-9230(03)00021-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12725900&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000182811200012&link_type=ISI) 8. Nilsson U , Jaensson M , Hugelius K , et al . A journey to a new stable state-further development of the postoperative recovery concept from day surgical perspective: a qualitative study. BMJ Open 2020;10:e037755. [doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2020-037755](http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-037755) 9. Royse CF . The patient’s surgical journey and consequences of poor recovery. Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol 2018;32:253–8. [doi:10.1016/j.bpa.2018.06.001](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bpa.2018.06.001) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=30522715&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 10. Deiner S , Liu X , Lin H-M , et al . Does Postoperative Cognitive Decline Result in New Disability After Surgery? Ann Surg 2021;274:e1108–14. [doi:10.1097/SLA.0000000000003764](http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/SLA.0000000000003764) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=32149824&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 11. Borchers F , Spies CD , Feinkohl I , et al . Methodology of measuring postoperative cognitive dysfunction: a systematic review. Br J Anaesth 2021;126:1119–27. [doi:10.1016/j.bja.2021.01.035](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bja.2021.01.035) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=33820655&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 12. Jessen F , Amariglio RE , Buckley RF , et al . The characterisation of subjective cognitive decline. Lancet Neurol 2020;19:271–8. [doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30368-0](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30368-0) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30368-0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=http://www.n&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 13. Curry LA , Krumholz HM , O’Cathain A , et al . Mixed methods in biomedical and health services research. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes 2013;6:119–23. [doi:10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.112.967885](http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.112.967885) [FREE Full Text](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiRlVMTCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6ODoiY2lyY2N2b3EiO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6NzoiNi8xLzExOSI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjI2OiIvYm1qb3Blbi8xNS8xL2UwOTM4NzIuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 14. Shorten A , Smith J . Mixed methods research: expanding the evidence base. Evid Based Nurs 2017;20:74–5. [doi:10.1136/eb-2017-102699](http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/eb-2017-102699) [FREE Full Text](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiRlVMTCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NjoiZWJudXJzIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjc6IjIwLzMvNzQiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoyNjoiL2Jtam9wZW4vMTUvMS9lMDkzODcyLmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 15. Schoonenboom J , Johnson RB . How to Construct a Mixed Methods Research Design. Kolner Z Soz Sozpsychol 2017;69:107–31. [doi:10.1007/s11577-017-0454-1](http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11577-017-0454-1) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s11577-017-0454-1&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=http://www.n&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 16. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5 5th ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013. 17. Nilsson U , Liander K , Rooyackers O , et al . Patients’ experiences of early postoperative cognition and its relation to cognitive decline and inflammatory responses: a protocol for a mixed-methods study. BMJ Open 2019;9:e032650. [doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2019-032650](http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2019-032650) 18. World Medical Association. Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 2013;310:2191–4. [doi:10.1001/jama.2013.281053](http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2013.281053) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/jama.2013.281053&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=24141714&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000327404400028&link_type=ISI) 19. Folstein MF , Folstein SE , McHugh PR . ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–98. [doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1202204&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1975AY27900005&link_type=ISI) 20. Moller JT , Cluitmans P , Rasmussen LS , et al . Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly ISPOCD1 study. ISPOCD investigators. International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction. Lancet 1998;351:857–61. [doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(97)07382-0](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(97)07382-0) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0140-6736(97)07382-0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9525362&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000072645500011&link_type=ISI) 21. Jaensson M , Dahlberg K , Eriksson M , et al . Evaluation of postoperative recovery in day surgery patients using a mobile phone application: a multicentre randomized trial. Br J Anaesth 2017;119:1030–8. [doi:10.1093/bja/aex331](http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/bja/aex331) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=29077818&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 22. Nilsson U , Dahlberg K , Jaensson M . The Swedish Web Version of the Quality of Recovery Scale Adapted for Use in a Mobile App: Prospective Psychometric Evaluation Study. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2017;5:e188. [doi:10.2196/mhealth.9061](http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/mhealth.9061) 23. Evered L , Silbert B , Scott DA , et al . Recommendations for a new perioperative cognitive impairment nomenclature. Alzheimer's & Dementia 2019;15:1115–6. [doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2019.05.005](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2019.05.005) 24. Elo S , Kyngäs H . The qualitative content analysis process. J Adv Nurs 2008;62:107–15. [doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x](http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18352969&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000254192000012&link_type=ISI) 25. Lezak MD , Howieson DB , Bigler ED , et al . Neuropsychological assessment. USA: OUP, 2012. 26. Fetters MD , Curry LA , Creswell JW . Achieving integration in mixed methods designs-principles and practices. Health Serv Res 2013;48:2134–56. [doi:10.1111/1475-6773.12117](http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1475-6773.12117) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/1475-6773.12117&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=24279835&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000327391900002&link_type=ISI) 27. Wu LM , Tanenbaum ML , Dijkers M , et al . Cognitive and neurobehavioral symptoms in patients with non-metastatic prostate cancer treated with androgen deprivation therapy or observation: A mixed methods study. Soc Sci Med 2016;156:80–9. [doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.03.016](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.03.016) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=27019142&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 28. Dijkstra JB , Jolles J . Postoperative cognitive dysfunction versus complaints: a discrepancy in long-term findings. Neuropsychol Rev 2002;12:1–14. [doi:10.1023/a:1015404122161](http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1015404122161) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1023/A:1015404122161&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12090716&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000176402700001&link_type=ISI) 29. Dijkstra JB , Houx PJ , Jolles J . Cognition after major surgery in the elderly: test performance and complaints. Br J Anaesth 1999;82:867–74. [doi:10.1093/bja/82.6.867](http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/bja/82.6.867) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/bja/82.6.867&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10562781&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000081159100012&link_type=ISI) 30. Moller JT , Svennild I , Johannessen NW , et al . Perioperative monitoring with pulse oximetry and late postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Br J Anaesth 1993;71:340–7. [doi:10.1093/bja/71.3.340](http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/bja/71.3.340) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/bja/71.3.340&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8398512&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1993LX19000003&link_type=ISI) 31. Makkar M , Hunter R , Kulkarni A , et al . Postoperative Cognitive Decline in Patients Undergoing Major Gynecologic Oncology Surgery: A Pilot Prospective Study. J Obstet Gynaecol Can 2024;46:102584. [doi:10.1016/j.jogc.2024.102584](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jogc.2024.102584) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=38878823&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 32. Earl Robertson F , Jacova C . A Systematic Review of Subjective Cognitive Characteristics Predictive of Longitudinal Outcomes in Older Adults. Gerontologist 2023;63:700–16. [doi:10.1093/geront/gnac109](http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnac109) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=35908232&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 33. Liu X , Yu Y , Zhu S . Inflammatory markers in postoperative delirium (POD) and cognitive dysfunction (POCD): A meta-analysis of observational studies. PLoS ONE 2018;13:e0195659. [doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0195659](http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195659) 34. De Cosmo G , Sessa F , Fiorini F , et al . Effect of remifentanil and fentanyl on postoperative cognitive function and cytokines level in elderly patients undergoing major abdominal surgery. J Clin Anesth 2016;35:40–6. [doi:10.1016/j.jclinane.2016.07.016](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinane.2016.07.016) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=27871564&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 35. Fu C , Lin J , Gong G , et al . Inflammatory markers in postoperative cognitive dysfunction for patients undergoing total hip arthroplasty: a meta-analysis. Aging Clin Exp Res 2022;34:277–88. [doi:10.1007/s40520-021-01919-7](http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40520-021-01919-7) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=34176086&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 36. Patel A , Zhang M , Liao G , et al . A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Examining the Impact of Age on Perioperative Inflammatory Biomarkers. Anesth Analg 2022;134:751–64. [doi:10.1213/ANE.0000000000005832](http://dx.doi.org/10.1213/ANE.0000000000005832) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=34962902&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 37. Clemmesen CG , Tavenier J , Andersen O , et al . Methylprednisolone and inflammatory stress response in older people undergoing surgery for hip fracture: a secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial. Eur Geriatr Med 2019;10:913–21. [doi:10.1007/s41999-019-00231-y](http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s41999-019-00231-y) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=34652763&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 38. Ryan PM , Scherry H , Pierson R , et al . NSAID use in orthopedic surgery: A review of current evidence and clinical practice guidelines. J Orthop Res 2024;42:707–16. [doi:10.1002/jor.25791](http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jor.25791) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=38273720&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 39. Yan Y , Guo TM , Zhu C . Effects of nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs on serum proinflammatory cytokines in the treatment of ankylosing spondylitis. Biochem Cell Biol 2018;96:450–6. [doi:10.1139/bcb-2017-0267](http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/bcb-2017-0267) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=29324184&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 40. Okinaka Y , Kageyama S , Goto T , et al . Metabolomic profiling of cancer-related fatigue involved in cachexia and chemotherapy. Sci Rep 2024;14:8329. [doi:10.1038/s41598-024-57747-y](http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-57747-y) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=38594321&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 41. Fosså A , Smeland KH , Fluge Ø , et al . Metabolic analysis of amino acids and vitamin B6 pathways in lymphoma survivors with cancer related chronic fatigue. PLoS One 2020;15:e0227384. [doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0227384](http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227384) 42. Christensen MHE , Fadnes DJ , Røst TH , et al . Inflammatory markers, the tryptophan-kynurenine pathway, and vitamin B status after bariatric surgery. PLoS One 2018;13:e0192169. [doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0192169](http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0192169) 43. Rubin GJ , Hardy R , Hotopf M . A systematic review and meta-analysis of the incidence and severity of postoperative fatigue. J Psychosom Res 2004;57:317–26. [doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00615-9](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00615-9) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00615-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15507259&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) [Web of Science](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000225443000016&link_type=ISI) 44. Cohen CL , Atkins KJ , Evered LA , et al . Examining Subjective Psychological Experiences of Postoperative Delirium in Older Cardiac Surgery Patients. Anesthesia & Analgesia 2022;06:06. [doi:10.1213/ANE.0000000000006226](http://dx.doi.org/10.1213/ANE.0000000000006226) 45. Toba MN , Malkinson TS , Howells H , et al . Same, Same but Different? A Multi-Method Review of the Processes Underlying Executive Control. Neuropsychol Rev 2024;34:418–54. [doi:10.1007/s11065-023-09577-4](http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-023-09577-4) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=36967445&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 46. Karlsson Å , Olofsson B , Stenvall M , et al . Older adults’ perspectives on rehabilitation and recovery one year after a hip fracture - a qualitative study. BMC Geriatr 2022;22:423. [doi:10.1186/s12877-022-03119-y](http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12877-022-03119-y) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1186/s12877-022-03119-y&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=35562681&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 47. Li L , Dohan D , Smith AK , et al . ‘It was a great brain, and I miss it’: lay perspectives on postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Br J Anaesth 2023;130:567–72. [doi:10.1016/j.bja.2023.02.003](http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bja.2023.02.003) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=36868965&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 48. Nilsson U , Dahlberg K , Jaensson M . Low Preoperative Mental and Physical Health is Associated with Poorer Postoperative Recovery in Patients Undergoing Day Surgery: A Secondary Analysis from a Randomized Controlled Study. World J Surg 2019;43:1949–56. [doi:10.1007/s00268-019-04995-z](http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00268-019-04995-z) [CrossRef](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s00268-019-04995-z&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=30937487&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 49. Jaensson M , Dahlberg K , Nilsson U . Factors influencing day surgery patients’ quality of postoperative recovery and satisfaction with recovery: a narrative review. Perioper Med (Lond) 2019;8:3. [doi:10.1186/s13741-019-0115-1](http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13741-019-0115-1) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=31139359&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 50. Ditton E , Johnson S , Hodyl N , et al . Improving Patient Outcomes Following Total Knee Arthroplasty: Identifying Rehabilitation Pathways Based on Modifiable Psychological Risk and Resilience Factors. Front Psychol 2020;11:1061. [doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01061](http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01061) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=32670136&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 51. Mogic L , Rutter EC , Tyas SL , et al . Functional social support and cognitive function in middle- and older-aged adults: a systematic review of cross-sectional and cohort studies. Syst Rev 2023;12:86. [doi:10.1186/s13643-023-02251-z](http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13643-023-02251-z) [PubMed](http://bmjopen.bmj.com/lookup/external-ref?access_num=37211612&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbmjopen%2F15%2F1%2Fe093872.atom) 52. Reza T , Grezenko H , Barker C , et al . Emotional Stress and Immune Response in Surgery: A Psychoneuroimmunological Perspective. Cureus 2023;15:e48727. [doi:10.7759/cureus.48727](http://dx.doi.org/10.7759/cureus.48727)